Relationship between circadian rhythm and brain cognitive functions

Shiyang XU, Miriam AKIOMA, Zhen YUAN

PDF(458 KB)
PDF(458 KB)
Front. Optoelectron. ›› 2021, Vol. 14 ›› Issue (3) : 278-287. DOI: 10.1007/s12200-021-1090-y
MINI REVIEW
MINI REVIEW

Relationship between circadian rhythm and brain cognitive functions

Author information +
History +

Abstract

Circadian rhythms are considered a masterstroke of natural selection, which gradually increase the adaptability of species to the Earth’s rotation. Importantly, the nervous system plays a key role in allowing organisms to maintain circadian rhythmicity. Circadian rhythms affect multiple aspects of cognitive functions (mainly via arousal), particularly those needed for effort-intensive cognitive tasks, which require considerable top-down executive control. These include inhibitory control, working memory, task switching, and psychomotor vigilance. This mini review highlights the recent advances in cognitive functioning in the optical and multimodal neuroimaging fields; it discusses the processing of brain cognitive functions during the circadian rhythm phase and the effects of the circadian rhythm on the cognitive component of the brain and the brain circuit supporting cognition.

Graphical abstract

Keywords

circadian rhythm / cognition / optical neuroimaging / multimodal neuroimaging

Cite this article

Download citation ▾
Shiyang XU, Miriam AKIOMA, Zhen YUAN. Relationship between circadian rhythm and brain cognitive functions. Front. Optoelectron., 2021, 14(3): 278‒287 https://doi.org/10.1007/s12200-021-1090-y

References

[1]
Refinetti R. Homeostasis and circadian rhythmicity in the control of body temperature. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1997, 813(1): 63–70
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[2]
Huang R C. The discoveries of molecular mechanisms for the circadian rhythm: The 2017 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine. Biomedical Journal, 2018, 41(1): 5–8
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[3]
DeMairan J. Histoire de l’Academie Royale des Sciences. Paris 1729
[4]
Reddy S, Reddy V, Sharma S. Physiology, Circadian Rhythm. In: StatPearls [Internet]. Treasure Island (FL): StatPearls Publishing, 2020
[5]
Burke T M, Scheer F A J L, Ronda J M, Czeisler C A, Wright K P Jr. Sleep inertia, sleep homeostatic and circadian influences on higher-order cognitive functions. Journal of Sleep Research, 2015, 24(4): 364–371
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[6]
Valdez P. Circadian rhythms in attention. Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine, 2019, 92(1): 81–92
Pubmed
[7]
Walker W H 2nd, Walton J C, DeVries A C, Nelson R J. Circadian rhythm disruption and mental health. Translational Psychiatry, 2020, 10(1): 28
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[8]
Bennett C L, Petros T V, Johnson M, Ferraro F R. Individual differences in the influence of time of day on executive functions. American Journal of Psychology, 2008, 121(3): 349–361
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[9]
Qasrawi S O, Pandi-Perumal S R, BaHammam A S. The effect of intermittent fasting during Ramadan on sleep, sleepiness, cognitive function, and circadian rhythm. Sleep and Breathing, 2017, 21(3): 577–586
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[10]
Owens J A. Sleep in children: cross-cultural perspectives. Sleep and Biological Rhythms, 2004, 2(3): 165–173
CrossRef Google scholar
[11]
Preckel F, Lipnevich A, Schneider S, Roberts R D. Chronotype, cognitive abilities, and academic achievement: a meta-analytic investigation. Learning and Individual Differences, 2011, 21(5): 483–492
CrossRef Google scholar
[12]
Ni Y, Wu L, Jiang J, Yang T, Wang Z, Ma L, Zheng L, Yang X, Wu Z, Fu Z. Late-night eating-induced physiological dysregulation and circadian misalignment are accompanied by microbial dysbiosis. Molecular Nutrition & Food Research, 2019, 63(24): e1900867
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[13]
Wehrens S M T, Christou S, Isherwood C, Middleton B, Gibbs M A, Archer S N, Skene D J, Johnston J D. Meal timing regulates the human circadian system. Current Biology, 2017, 27(12): 1768–1775.e3
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[14]
Shi L, Liu Y, Jiang T, Yan P, Cao F, Chen Y, Wei H, Liu J. Relationship between mental health, the CLOCK gene, and sleep quality in surgical nurses: a cross-sectional study. BioMed Research International, 2020, 4795763
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[15]
Kim H Y, Seo K, Jeon H J, Lee U, Lee H. Application of functional near-infrared spectroscopy to the study of brain function in humans and animal models. Molecules and Cells, 2017, 40(8): 523–532
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[16]
Huettel S A, Song A W, McCarthy G. Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Sunderland: Sinauer Associates, 2004
[17]
Boas D A, Brooks D H, Miller E L, DiMarzio C A, Kilmer M, Gaudette R J, Zhang Q. Imaging the body with diffuse optical tomography. IEEE Signal Processing Magazine, 2001, 18(6): 57–75
CrossRef Google scholar
[18]
Niedermeyer E, da Silva F H L. Electroencephalography: Basic Principles, Clinical Applications, and Related Fields. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2005
[19]
Anderson J R. Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications. New York: Worth Publishers, 2005
[20]
Cohen R A, Sparling-Cohen Y A, O’Donnell B F. The Neuropsychology of Attention. New York: Springer, 1993
[21]
Graw P, Kräuchi K, Knoblauch V, Wirz-Justice A, Cajochen C. Circadian and wake-dependent modulation of fastest and slowest reaction times during the psychomotor vigilance task. Physiology & Behavior, 2004, 80(5): 695–701
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[22]
Lim J, Dinges D F. Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 2008, 1129(1): 305–322
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[23]
Folkard S. Diurnal variation in logical reasoning. British Journal of Psychology, 1975, 66(1): 1–8
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[24]
Cohen R A. Neural mechanisms of attention. In: Cohen R A, Sparling-Cohen Y A, O’Donnell B F, eds. The Neuropsychology of Attention. New York: Springer, 2014, 211–264
[25]
Sturm W, Willmes K. On the functional neuroanatomy of intrinsic and phasic alertness. NeuroImage, 2001, 14(1 Pt 2): S76–S84
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[26]
Gazzaley A, Rissman J, Cooney J, Rutman A, Seibert T, Clapp W, D’Esposito M. Functional interactions between prefrontal and visual association cortex contribute to top-down modulation of visual processing. Cerebral Cortex, 2007, 17(Suppl 1): i125–i135
Pubmed
[27]
Morecraft R J, Geula C, Mesulam M M. Architecture of connectivity within a cingulo-fronto-parietal neurocognitive network for directed attention. Archives of Neurology, 1993, 50(3): 279–284
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[28]
Soshi T, Kuriyama K, Aritake S, Enomoto M, Hida A, Tamura M, Kim Y, Mishima K. Sleep deprivation influences diurnal variation of human time perception with prefrontal activity change: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. PLoS One, 2010, 5(1): e8395
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[29]
Valdez P, Ramírez C, García A, Talamantes J, Armijo P, Borrani J. Circadian rhythms in components of attention. Biological Rhythm Research, 2005, 36(1–2): 57–65
CrossRef Google scholar
[30]
Riley M. Musical Listening in the German Enlightenment: Attention, Wonder and Astonishment. London: Taylor & Francis, 2017
[31]
Matchock R L, Mordkoff J T. Chronotype and time-of-day influences on the alerting, orienting, and executive components of attention. Experimental Brain Research, 2009, 192(2): 189–198
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[32]
Nicholls C, Bruno R, Matthews A. Chronic cannabis use and ERP correlates of visual selective attention during the performance of a flanker go/nogo task. Biological Psychology, 2015, 110: 115–125
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[33]
Valdez P, Ramírez C, García A, Talamantes J, Cortez J. Circadian and homeostatic variation in sustained attention. Chronobiology International, 2010, 27(2): 393–416
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[34]
Johnson S W, North R A. Opioids excite dopamine neurons by hyperpolarization of local interneurons. Journal of Neuroscience, 1992, 12(2): 483–488
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[35]
Carrier J, Monk T H. Circadian rhythms of performance: new trends. Chronobiology International, 2000, 17(6): 719–732
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[36]
Wright K P Jr, Hull J T, Czeisler C A. Relationship between alertness, performance, and body temperature in humans. American Journal of Physiology. Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 2002, 283(6): R1370–R1377
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[37]
Bonnefond A, Rohmer O, Hoeft A, Muzet A, Tassi P. Interaction of age with time of day and mental load in different cognitive tasks. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2003, 96(3 Pt 2): 1223–1236
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[38]
Babkoff H, Caspy T, Mikulincer M, Sing H C. Monotonic and rhythmic influences: a challenge for sleep deprivation research. Psychological Bulletin, 1991, 109(3): 411–428
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[39]
Sagaspe P, Sanchez-Ortuno M, Charles A, Taillard J, Valtat C, Bioulac B, Philip P. Effects of sleep deprivation on Color-Word, Emotional, and Specific Stroop interference and on self-reported anxiety. Brain and Cognition, 2006, 60(1): 76–87
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[40]
Krishnan H C, Lyons L C. Synchrony and desynchrony in circadian clocks: impacts on learning and memory. Learning & Memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.), 2015, 22(9): 426–437
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[41]
Dinges D F, Pack F, Williams K, Gillen K A, Powell J W, Ott G E, Aptowicz C, Pack A I. Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4–5 hours per night. Sleep, 1997, 20(4): 267–277
Pubmed
[42]
Gillberg M, Åkerstedt T. Sleep loss and performance: no “safe” duration of a monotonous task. Physiology & Behavior, 1998, 64(5): 599–604
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[43]
McCarthy M E, Waters W F. Decreased attentional responsivity during sleep deprivation: orienting response latency, amplitude, and habituation. Sleep, 1997, 20(2): 115–123
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[44]
De Gennaro L, Ferrara M, Curcio G, Bertini M. Visual search performance across 40 h of continuous wakefulness: Measures of speed and accuracy and relation with oculomotor performance. Physiology & Behavior, 2001, 74(1–2): 197–204
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[45]
Chee Y L, Crawford J C, Watson H G, Greaves M. Guidelines on the assessment of bleeding risk prior to surgery or invasive procedures. British Journal of Haematology, 2008, 140(5): 496–504
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[46]
Drummond S P A, Brown G G. The effects of total sleep deprivation on cerebral responses to cognitive performance. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2001, 25(5 Suppl 1): S68–S73
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[47]
Tomasi D, Wang R L, Telang F, Boronikolas V, Jayne M C, Wang G J, Fowler J S, Volkow N D. Impairment of attentional networks after 1 night of sleep deprivation. Cerebral Cortex, 2009, 19(1): 233–240
Pubmed
[48]
Chee M W L, Goh C S F, Namburi P, Parimal S, Seidl K N, Kastner S. Effects of sleep deprivation on cortical activation during directed attention in the absence and presence of visual stimuli. NeuroImage, 2011, 58(2): 595–604
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[49]
De Havas J A, Parimal S, Soon C S, Chee M W. Sleep deprivation reduces default mode network connectivity and anti-correlation during rest and task performance. NeuroImage, 2012, 59(2): 1745–1751
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[50]
Muto V, Jaspar M, Meyer C, Kussé C, Chellappa S L, Degueldre C, Balteau E, Shaffii-Le Bourdiec A, Luxen A, Middleton B, Archer S N, Phillips C, Collette F, Vandewalle G, Dijk D J, Maquet P. Local modulation of human brain responses by circadian rhythmicity and sleep debt. Science, 2016, 353(6300): 687–690
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[51]
Miyake A, Shah P. Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1999, 506
[52]
Baddeley A. The fractionation of working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 1996, 93(24): 13468–13472
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[53]
Awh E, Jonides J, Smith E E, Buxton R B, Frank L R, Love T, Wong E C, Gmeindl L. Rehearsal in spatial working memory: evidence from neuroimaging. Psychological Science, 1999, 10(5): 433–437
CrossRef Google scholar
[54]
Stern C E, Sherman S J, Kirchhoff B A, Hasselmo M E. Medial temporal and prefrontal contributions to working memory tasks with novel and familiar stimuli. Hippocampus, 2001, 11(4): 337–346
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[55]
McDowell S, Whyte J, D’Esposito M. Working memory impairments in traumatic brain injury: evidence from a dual-task paradigm. Neuropsychologia, 1997, 35(10): 1341–1353
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[56]
Gerstner J R, Yin J C P. Circadian rhythms and memory formation. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 2010, 11(8): 577–588
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[57]
Domagalik A, Oginska H, Beldzik E, Fafrowicz M, Pokrywka M, Chaniecki P, Marek T. Long-term reduction of short wavelength light affects sustained attention and visuospatial working memory. bioRxiv, 2019, 581314
CrossRef Google scholar
[58]
Laird D A. Relative performance of college students as conditioned by time of day and day of week. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1925, 8(1): 50–63
CrossRef Google scholar
[59]
Vedhara K, Hyde J, Gilchrist I D, Tytherleigh M, Plummer S. Acute stress, memory, attention and cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 2000, 25(6): 535–549
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[60]
Potter P, Wolf L, Boxerman S, Grayson D, Sledge J, Dunagan C, Evanoff B. Understanding the cognitive work of nursing in the acute care environment. Journal of Nursing Administration, 2005, 35(7–8): 327–335
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[61]
Blake M J F. Time of day effects on performance in a range of tasks. Psychonomic Science, 1967, 9(6): 349–350
CrossRef Google scholar
[62]
Rowe G, Hasher L, Turcotte J. Age and synchrony effects in visuospatial working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2009, 62(10): 1873–1880
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[63]
Folkard S. Time of day and level of processing. Memory & Cognition, 1979, 7(4): 247–252
CrossRef Google scholar
[64]
Lewandowska K, Wachowicz B, Marek T, Oginska H, Fafrowicz M. Would you say “yes” in the evening? Time-of-day effect on response bias in four types of working memory recognition tasks. Chronobiology International, 2018, 35(1): 80–89
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[65]
Chee M W L, Choo W C. Functional imaging of working memory after 24 hr of total sleep deprivation. Journal of Neuroscience, 2004, 24(19): 4560–4567
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[66]
Choo W C, Lee W W, Venkatraman V, Sheu F S, Chee M W. Dissociation of cortical regions modulated by both working memory load and sleep deprivation and by sleep deprivation alone. NeuroImage, 2005, 25(2): 579–587
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[67]
Mu Q, Mishory A, Johnson K A, Nahas Z, Kozel F A, Yamanaka K, Bohning D E, George M S. Decreased brain activation during a working memory task at rested baseline is associated with vulnerability to sleep deprivation. Sleep, 2005, 28(4): 433–448
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[68]
Chee M W L, Chuah L Y M, Venkatraman V, Chan W Y, Philip P, Dinges D F. Functional imaging of working memory following normal sleep and after 24 and 35 h of sleep deprivation: Correlations of fronto-parietal activation with performance. NeuroImage, 2006, 31(1): 419–428
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[69]
Lim J, Choo W C, Chee M W L. Reproducibility of changes in behaviour and fMRI activation associated with sleep deprivation in a working memory task. Sleep (Basel), 2007, 30(1): 61–70
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[70]
Honma M, Soshi T, Kim Y, Kuriyama K. Right prefrontal activity reflects the ability to overcome sleepiness during working memory tasks: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. PLoS One, 2010, 5(9): e12923
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[71]
McKenna B S, Eyler L T. Overlapping prefrontal systems involved in cognitive and emotional processing in euthymic bipolar disorder and following sleep deprivation: a review of functional neuroimaging studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 2012, 32(7): 650–663
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[72]
Thomas R J, Rosen B R, Stern C E, Weiss J W, Kwong K K. Functional imaging of working memory in obstructive sleep-disordered breathing. Journal of Applied Physiology, 2005, 98(6): 2226–2234
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[73]
McKenna B S, Sutherland A N, Legenkaya A P, Eyler L T. Abnormalities of brain response during encoding into verbal working memory among euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 2014, 16(3): 289–299
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[74]
Drummond S P A, Walker M, Almklov E, Campos M, Anderson D E, Straus L D. Neural correlates of working memory performance in primary insomnia. Sleep (Basel), 2013, 36(9): 1307–1316
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[75]
Stroop J R. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 1992, 121(1): 15–23
CrossRef Google scholar
[76]
Hartley L R, Shirley E. Color-name interference at different times of day. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1976, 61(1): 119–122
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[77]
Manly T, Lewis G H, Robertson I H, Watson P C, Datta A. Coffee in the cornflakes: time-of-day as a modulator of executive response control. Neuropsychologia, 2002, 40(1): 1–6
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[78]
Harrison Y, Jones K, Waterhouse J. The influence of time awake and circadian rhythm upon performance on a frontal lobe task. Neuropsychologia, 2007, 45(8): 1966–1972
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[79]
Bratzke D, Steinborn M B, Rolke B, Ulrich R. Effects of sleep loss and circadian rhythm on executive inhibitory control in the Stroop and Simon tasks. Chronobiology International, 2012, 29(1): 55–61
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[80]
Schmidt C, Peigneux P, Leclercq Y, Sterpenich V, Vandewalle G, Phillips C, Berthomier P, Berthomier C, Tinguely G, Gais S, Schabus M, Desseilles M, Dang-Vu T, Salmon E, Degueldre C, Balteau E, Luxen A, Cajochen C, Maquet P, Collette F. Circadian preference modulates the neural substrate of conflict processing across the day. PLoS One, 2012, 7(1): e29658
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[81]
Miller E K, Cohen J D. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 2001, 24(1): 167–202
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[82]
Dove A, Pollmann S, Schubert T, Wiggins C J, von Cramon D Y. Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: an event-related fMRI study. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 2000, 9(1): 103–109
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[83]
Heuer H, Kleinsorge T, Klein W, Kohlisch O. Total sleep deprivation increases the costs of shifting between simple cognitive tasks. Acta Psychologica, 2004, 117(1): 29–64
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[84]
Bratzke D, Rolke B, Steinborn M B, Ulrich R. The effect of 40 h constant wakefulness on task-switching efficiency. Journal of Sleep Research, 2009, 18(2): 167–172
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[85]
Shinkai S, Watanabe S, Kurokawa Y, Torii J. Salivary cortisol for monitoring circadian rhythm variation in adrenal activity during shiftwork. International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health, 1993, 64(7): 499–502
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[86]
Ramírez C, García A, Valdez P. Identification of circadian rhythms in cognitive inhibition and flexibility using a Stroop task. Sleep and Biological Rhythms, 2012, 10(2): 136–144
CrossRef Google scholar
[87]
Mednick S A. The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 1962, 69(3): 220–232
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[88]
Sherman S M, Mumford J A, Schnyer D M. Hippocampal activity mediates the relationship between circadian activity rhythms and memory in older adults. Neuropsychologia, 2015, 75: 617–625
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[89]
May C P. Synchrony effects in cognition: the costs and a benefit. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1999, 6(1): 142–147
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[90]
Wieth M B, Zacks R T. Time of day effects on problem solving: When the non-optimal is optimal. Thinking & Reasoning, 2011, 17(4): 387–401
CrossRef Google scholar
[91]
West R, Murphy K J, Armilio M L, Craik F I, Stuss D T. Effects of time of day on age differences in working memory. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 2002, 57(1): 3–10
CrossRef Pubmed Google scholar
[92]
Lu F M, Yuan Z. PET/SPECT molecular imaging in clinical neuroscience: recent advances in the investigation of CNS diseases. Quantitative Imaging in Medicine and Surgery, 2015, 5(3): 433–447
Pubmed
[93]
Chen B, Moreland J, Zhang J. Human brain functional MRI and DTI visualization with virtual reality. Quantitative Imaging in Medicine and Surgery, 2011, 1(1): 11–16
Pubmed

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by MYRG2019-00082-FHS and MYRG2018-00081-FHS Grants from the University of Macau, as well as FDCT 025/2015/A1 and FDCT 0011/2018/A1 Grants from the Macau Government.

Electronic Supplementary Material

Supplementary material is available in the online version of this article at https://doi.org/10.1007/s12200-021-1090-y and is accessible for authorized users.

RIGHTS & PERMISSIONS

2021 Higher Education Press
AI Summary AI Mindmap
PDF(458 KB)

Accesses

Citations

Detail

Sections
Recommended

/