Affect and satisfaction time dependency: An experience sampling study

Petra Anić, Marko Tončić

PDF
Psych Journal ›› 2024, Vol. 13 ›› Issue (3) : 456-464. DOI: 10.1002/pchj.763
ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Affect and satisfaction time dependency: An experience sampling study

Author information +
History +

Abstract

The aim of this study was to test the time dependency between affect and satisfaction on a momentary level. Ninety-eight students participated in the study, using the experience sampling method. Affect and satisfaction scales were administered five times a day for 7 days via handheld devices, sampling the whole awake period with ratings approximately 3–4 h apart. The aim of this study was to examine the cross-correlation between affect and satisfaction at the intra-individual level and to test their temporal consistency via lagged cross-correlations. On average, satisfaction was robustly associated with positive affect (PA; mean correlation 0.50) and negative affect (NA; mean correlation −0.38). The correlation of satisfaction with affect factors showed a consistent temporal dependency. Lag (i.e., the shift of one time series with respect to another) significantly affected the magnitude of the correlation coefficients of satisfaction with PA and NA (explaining almost half of the correlation variance). A significant affect–satisfaction cross-correlation can be found when no lag is present. The introduction of a lag reduces the affect–satisfaction cross-correlation to virtually zero. Research suggests that affect and satisfaction overlap at the momentary level, and the results of this study imply that they are also time-dependent. These findings corroborate the idea that momentary satisfaction judgments are partially based on available emotional information, both in terms of intensity and temporal consistency.

Keywords

cross-correlation / momentary affect / satisfaction / time-dependency

Cite this article

Download citation ▾
Petra Anić, Marko Tončić. Affect and satisfaction time dependency: An experience sampling study. Psych Journal, 2024, 13(3): 456‒464 https://doi.org/10.1002/pchj.763

References

[1]
Andrews, F. M., & Robinson, J. P. (1991). Measures of subjective well-being. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes (pp. 61–114). Academic Press.
CrossRef Google scholar
[2]
Arthaud-day, M. L., Rode, J. C., Mooney, C. H., & Near, J. P. (2005). The subjective well-being construct: A test of its convergent, discriminant, and factorial validity. Social Indicators Research, 74(3), 445–476.
CrossRef Google scholar
[3]
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182.
CrossRef Google scholar
[4]
Barrett, L. F., & Barrett, D. J. (2001). An introduction to computerized experience sampling in psychology. Social Science Computer Review, 19(2), 175–185.
CrossRef Google scholar
[5]
Bonito, J. A., Ruppel, E. K., & Keyton, J. (2012). Reliability estimates for multilevel designs in group research. Small Group Research, 43(4), 443–467.
CrossRef Google scholar
[6]
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(4), 668–678.
CrossRef Google scholar
[7]
Diener, E. (2000). Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and a proposal for a national index. American Psychologist, 55(1), 34–43.
CrossRef Google scholar
[8]
Diener, E., Fujita, F., Tay, L., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2012). Purpose, mood, and pleasure in predicting satisfaction judgments. Social Indicators Research, 105(3), 333–341.
CrossRef Google scholar
[9]
Diener, E., Heintzelman, S. J., Kushlev, K., Tay, L., Wirtz, D., Lutes, L. D., & Oishi, S. (2017). Findings all psychologists should know from the new science on subjective well-being. Canadian Psychology, 58(2), 87–104.
CrossRef Google scholar
[10]
Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (1999). Personality and subjective well-being. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 213–229). Russell Sage Foundation.
[11]
Diener, E., Ng, W., Harter, J., & Arora, R. (2010). Wealth and happiness across the world: Material prosperity predicts life evaluation, whereas psychosocial prosperity predicts positive feeling. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(1), 52–61.
CrossRef Google scholar
[12]
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125(2), 276–302.
CrossRef Google scholar
[13]
Fahrenberg, J., Myrtek, M., Pawlik, K., & Perrez, M. (2007). Ambulatory assessment – monitoring behavior in daily life settings. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23(4), 206–213.
CrossRef Google scholar
[14]
Feldman Barrett, L., & Russell, J. A. (1998). Independence and bipolarity in the structure of current affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(4), 967–984.
CrossRef Google scholar
[15]
Fredrickson, B. L. (2000). Extracting meaning from past affective experiences: The importance of peaks, ends, and specific emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 14(4), 577–606.
CrossRef Google scholar
[16]
Fujita, F., & Diener, E. (2005). Life satisfaction set point: Stability and change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(1), 158–164.
CrossRef Google scholar
[17]
Hudson, N. W., Lucas, R. E., & Donnellan, M. B. (2017). Day-to-day affect is surprisingly stable. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8(1), 45–54.
CrossRef Google scholar
[18]
Jayawickreme, E., Forgeard, M. J. C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2012). The engine of well-being. Review of General Psychology, 16(4), 327–342.
CrossRef Google scholar
[19]
Jayawickreme, E., Tsukayama, E., Blackie, L. E. R., & Weiss, B. (2021). Examining within-person relationships between state assessments of affect and eudaimonic well-being using multi-level structural equation modeling. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 16(5), 691–700.
CrossRef Google scholar
[20]
Jayawickreme, E., Tsukayama, E., & Kashdan, T. B. (2017a). Examining the effect of affect on life satisfaction judgments: A within-person perspective. Journal of Research in Personality, 68(April), 32–37.
CrossRef Google scholar
[21]
Jayawickreme, E., Tsukayama, E., & Kashdan, T. B. (2017b). Examining the within-person effect of affect on daily satisfaction. Journal of Research in Personality, 71(September), 27–32.
CrossRef Google scholar
[22]
Jovanovic, V. (2011). Personality and subjective well-being: One neglected model of personality and two forgotten aspects of subjective well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(5), 631–635.
CrossRef Google scholar
[23]
Kardum, I. (1998). Affect intensity and frequency: Their relation to mean level and variability of positive and negative affect and Eysencks personality traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 26(1), 33–47.
CrossRef Google scholar
[24]
Kashdan, T. B., & Nezlek, J. B. (2012). Whether, when, and how is spirituality related to well-being? Moving beyond single occasion questionnaires to understanding daily process. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(11), 1523–1535.
CrossRef Google scholar
[25]
Kuppens, P., Realo, A., & Diener, E. (2008). The role of positive and negative emotions in life satisfaction judgment across nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(1), 66–75.
CrossRef Google scholar
[26]
Larsen, R. J. (1987). The stability of mood variability: A spectral analytic approach to daily mood assessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1195–1204.
CrossRef Google scholar
[27]
Larsen, R. J. (2009). The contribution of positive and negative affect to emotional well-being. Psychological Topics, 18, 247–266.
[28]
Larsen, R. J., Augustine, A. A., & Prizmic, Z. (2009). A process approach to emotion and personality: Using time as a facet of data. Cognition & Emotion, 23(7), 1407–1426.
CrossRef Google scholar
[29]
Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., Barrett, L. F., Liquist, K. A., & Wagner, T. D. (2012). The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(3), 121–143.
CrossRef Google scholar
[30]
Lucas, R. E., Donnellan, B. M., & Brent Donnellan, M. (2012). Estimating the reliability of single-item life satisfaction measures: Results from four National Panel Studies. Social Indicators Research, 105(3), 323–331.
CrossRef Google scholar
[31]
Lucas, R. E., & Donnellan, M. B. (2007). How stable is happiness? Using the STARTS model to estimate the stability of life satisfaction. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(5), 1091–1098.
CrossRef Google scholar
[32]
Lucas, R. E., Freedman, V. A., & Cornman, J. C. (2018). The short-term stability of life satisfaction judgments. Emotion, 18(7), 1024–1031.
CrossRef Google scholar
[33]
Luhmann, M., Hawkley, L. C., Eid, M., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2012). Time frames and the distinction between affective and cognitive well-being. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(4), 431–441.
CrossRef Google scholar
[34]
Luhmann, M., Krasko, J., & Terwiel, S. (2021). Subjective well-being as a dynamic construct. In The handbook of personality dynamics and processes (pp. 1231–1249). Elsevier.
CrossRef Google scholar
[35]
Merz, E. L., & Roesch, S. C. (2011). Modeling trait and state variation using multilevel factor analysis with PANAS daily diary data. Journal of Research in Personality, 45(1), 2–9.
CrossRef Google scholar
[36]
Mitsutake, G., Otsuka, K., Cornélissen, G., Herold, M., Günther, R., Dawes, C., Burch, J. B., Watson, D., & Halberg, F. (2001). Circadian and infradian rhythms in mood. Biomedicine & Pharmacotherapy, 55, 94–100.
CrossRef Google scholar
[37]
Nezlek, J. B. (2005). Distinguishing affective and non-affective reactions to daily events. Journal of Personality, 73, 1539–1568.
CrossRef Google scholar
[38]
Nezlek, J. B. (2012). Diary methods for social and personality psychology. In J. B. Nezlek (Ed.), The SAGE library of methods in social and personality psychology. Sage Publications.
[39]
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford University Press.
[40]
Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (1993). Review of the satisfaction with life scale. Psychological Assessment, 5(2), 164–172.
CrossRef Google scholar
[41]
Pavot, W., Diener, E., & Suh, E. (1998). The temporal satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 70(2), 340–354.
CrossRef Google scholar
[42]
Redelmeier, D. A., & Kahneman, D. (1996). Patients' memories of painful medical treatments: Real-time and retrospective evaluations of two minimally invasive procedures. Pain, 66(1), 3–8.
CrossRef Google scholar
[43]
Revelle, W., & Wilt, J. (2019). Analyzing dynamic data: A tutorial. Personality and Individual Differences, 136(1), 38–51.
CrossRef Google scholar
[44]
Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Episodic and semantic knowledge in emotional self-report: Evidence for two judgment processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1), 198–215.
CrossRef Google scholar
[45]
Ross, M., Eyman, A., & Kischuck, N. (1986). Determinants of subjective well- being. In J. M. Olson, C. P. Herman, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Relative deprivation and social comparison: The Ontario symposium (pp. 79–93). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
[46]
Sagiv, L., & Schwartz, S. H. (2000). Value priorities and subjective well-being: Direct relations and congruity effects. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30(2), 177–198.
CrossRef Google scholar
[47]
Sailer, U., Rosenberg, P., Nima, A., Gamble, A., Gärling, T., Archer, T., & Garcia, D. (2014). A happier and less sinister past, a more hedonistic and less fatalistic present and a more structured future: Time perspective and well-being. PeerJ, 2, e303.
CrossRef Google scholar
[48]
Schimmack, U. (2002). Methodological issues in the assessment of the affective component of subjective well-being. In A. Ohn & M. van Dulmen (Eds.), Handbook of methods in positive psychology (pp. 96–110). Oxford University Press.
[49]
Schimmack, U., & Anusic, I. (2016). Stability and change of personality traits, self-esteem, and well-being: Introducing the meta-analytic stability and change model of retest correlations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(5), 766–781.
[50]
Schimmack, U., Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2002). Life-satisfaction is a momentary judgment and a stable personality characteristic: The use of chronically accessible and stable sources. Journal of Personality, 70(3), 345–384.
CrossRef Google scholar
[51]
Schimmack, U., Krause, P., Wagner, G. G., & Schupp, J. (2009). Stability and change of well being: An experimentally enhanced latent state-trait-error analysis. Social Indicators Research, 95(1), 19–31.
CrossRef Google scholar
[52]
Schimmack, U., & Oishi, S. (2005). The influence of chronically and temporarily accessible information on life satisfaction judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(3), 395–406.
CrossRef Google scholar
[53]
Schimmack, U., Radhakrishnan, P., Oishi, S., Dzokoto, V., & Ahadi, S. (2002). Culture, personality, and subjective well-being: Integrating process models of life satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(4), 582–593.
CrossRef Google scholar
[54]
Schwarz, N. (2010). Feelings-as-information theory. In P. van Lange, A. Kruglansky, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (pp. 289–307). Sage Publications.
[55]
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of Weil-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(3), 513–523.
CrossRef Google scholar
[56]
Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1999). Reports of subjective well-being: Judgmental processes and their methodological implications. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 61–84). Russell Sage Foundation.
[57]
Shrout, P., & Lane, S. P. (2012). Psychometrics. In M. R. Mehl & T. S. Conner (Eds.), Handbook of research methods for studying daily life (pp. 302–320). Guilford Press.
[58]
Steger, M. F., Kashdan, T. B., & Oishi, S. (2008). Being good by doing good: Daily eudaimonic activity and well-being. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(1), 22–42.
CrossRef Google scholar
[59]
Stone, A. A., & Broderick, J. E. (2007). Real-time data collection for pain: Appraisal and current status. Pain Medicine, 8(suppl 3), S85–S93.
CrossRef Google scholar
[60]
Stone, A. A., & Shiffman, S. (2002). Capturing momentary, self-report data: A proposal for reporting guidelines. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 24(3), 236–243.
CrossRef Google scholar
[61]
Stone, A. A., Shiffman, S., Schwartz, J. E., Broderick, J. E., & Hufford, M. R. (2003). Patient compliance with paper and electronic diaries. Controlled Clinical Trials, 24(2), 182–199.
CrossRef Google scholar
[62]
Suh, E. M., Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1996). Events and subjective well-being: Only recent events matter. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(5), 1091–1102.
CrossRef Google scholar
[63]
Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Wilcox, K. J., Segal, N. L., & Rich, S. (1988). Personality similarity in twins reared apart and together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1031–1039.
CrossRef Google scholar
[64]
Thayer, R. E. (1989). The biopsychology of mood and arousal. Oxford University Press.
[65]
Tončić, M., & Anić, P. (2020). Effects of momentary affect on satisfaction judgments. Journal of Individual Differences, 41(2), 61–67.
CrossRef Google scholar
[66]
Tončić, M., Brdar, I., & Anić, P. (2010). Ličnost i zadovoljstvo životom: Kakva je uloga pozitivnih i negativnih emocija? Personality and life satisfaction: What's the role of positive and negative emotions? In I. Sorić, V. Ćubela Adorić, L. Gregov, & Z. Penezić (Eds.), 17. Dani psihologije.
[67]
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070.
CrossRef Google scholar
[68]
Yik, M. S. M., Russell, J. A., & Suzuki, N. (2003). Relating momentary affect to the five factor model of personality: A Japanese case. Japanese Psychological Research, 45(2), 80–93.
CrossRef Google scholar
[69]
Zelenski, J. M., & Larsen, R. J. (2000). The distribution of basic emotions in everyday life: A state and trait perspective from experience sampling data. Journal of Research in Personality, 34(2), 178–197.
CrossRef Google scholar

RIGHTS & PERMISSIONS

2024 2024 The Authors. PsyCh Journal published by Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences and John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd.
PDF

Accesses

Citations

Detail

Sections
Recommended

/