Scarcity makes people short-sighted? Evidence from intertemporal decision-making
Chang Zeng, Qi Wu, Cuihua Bi, Huaiyuan Qi
Scarcity makes people short-sighted? Evidence from intertemporal decision-making
With the acceleration of the pace of society, the problem of scarcity of time resources is becoming more prominent, which may lead to short-sighted behavior. This study aimed to examine the impact of time scarcity on intertemporal choice and its underlying mechanisms through three experiments. Study 1 aimed to investigate the causal relationship between time scarcity and intertemporal choice and the mediating role of anxiety between time scarcity and intertemporal choice. The results showed that participants with a stronger perception of time scarcity were more likely to choose smaller and sooner gains, and that the mediating role of anxiety was significant. In Study 2, the control of time was introduced to explore whether the sense of control could alleviate the negative emotion caused by time scarcity. The results showed that individuals with high levels of control had significantly less anxiety under higher time-scarcity conditions than those with low levels of control. In Study 3, we manipulated time scarcity and found that the sense of control moderated the mediating effect of anxiety in time scarcity and intertemporal choice. Overall, these findings suggest that the sense of control over time is associated with less anxiety from time scarcity, which in turn fosters preferences for delayed gains.
anxiety / intertemporal choice / sense of control / time scarcity
[1] |
Ashman, O., Shiomura, K., & Levy, B. R. (2006). Influence of culture and age on control beliefs: The missing link of interdependence. International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 62(2), 143–157.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[2] |
Astakhova, M. N. (2015). The curvilinear relationship between work passion and organizational citizenship behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 130, 361-374.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[3] |
Barlow, D. H. (2002). Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treatment of anxiety and panic. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(8), 1453.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[4] |
Bernaola, D., Willows, G. D., & West, D. (2021). The relevance of anger, anxiety, gender and race in investment decisions. Mind & Society, 20(1), 1–21.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[5] |
Bye, D., & Pushkar, D. (2009). How need for cognition and perceived control are differentially linked to emotional outcomes in the transition to retirement. Motivation & Emotion, 33(3), 320–332.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[6] |
Cao, Y., & Liu, Z. (2015). Factor structure and factorial invariance of the state-trait anxiety inventory for Chinese children and adolescents. PsyCh Journal, 4(2), 74–87.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[7] |
Chen, X. X., & He, G. B. (2014). Stress makes people short-sighted? Evidence from intertemporal decision making. Applied Psychology, 20(1), 3–10.
|
[8] |
Cohen, J. B., Pham, M. T., & Andrade, E. B. (2013). The nature and role of affect in consumer behavior. Social Science Electronic Publishing, 12(3): 188–240.
|
[9] |
Daniel, K. (2003). Maps of bounded rationality: Psychology for behavioral economics. American Economic Review, 93(5), 1449–1475.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[10] |
De Wit, H., Flory, J. D., Acheson, A., McCloskey, M., & Manuck, S. B. (2007). IQ and nonplanning impulsivity are independently associated with delay discounting in middle-aged adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(1), 111–121.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[11] |
Devoe, S. E., & Pfeffer, J. (2011). Time is tight: How higher economic value of time increases feelings of time pressure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(4), 665–676.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[12] |
Duan, Y. (2014). Money improves the sense of control level (master's thesis). Hunan Normal University.
|
[13] |
Evans, J. S., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 8(3), 223–241.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[14] |
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[15] |
Figner, B., Knoch, D., Johnson, E. J., Krosch, A. R., Lisanby, S. H., Fehr, E., & Weber, E. U. (2010). Lateral prefrontal cortex and self-control in intertemporal choice. Nature Neuroscience, 13(5), 538–539.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[16] |
Frederick, S., Loewenstein, G., & O'Donoghue, T. (2002). Time discounting and time preference: A critical review. Journal of Economic Literature, 40(2), 351–401.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[17] |
Fritsche, I., Jonas, E., & Fankhänel, T. (2008). The role of control motivation in mortality salience effects on ingroup support and defense. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(3), 524–541.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[18] |
Giurge, L. M., Whillans, A. V., & West, C. (2020). Why time poverty matters for individuals, organisations and nations. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(10), 993–1003.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[19] |
Greenaway, K. H., Haslam, S. A., Cruwys, T., Branscombe, N. R., Ysseldyk, R., & Heldreth, C. (2015). From "we" to "me": Group identification enhances perceived personal control with consequences for health and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(1), 53–74.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[20] |
Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., & Rangel, A. (2009). Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science, 324(5927), 646–648.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[21] |
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach (p. 20). Guilford Publications.
|
[22] |
Huang, Y. Z., Jiang, J. Q., Yao, N. S., Zhao, L., & Qian, M. Y. (2019). Self-focused attention and social anxiety: The mediating role of sense of control. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 27(6), 1232–1236.
|
[23] |
Kay, A. C., Whitson, J. A., Gaucher, D., & Galinsky, A. D. (2009). Compensatory control: Achieving order through the mind, our institutions, and the heavens. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(5), 264–268.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[24] |
Kim, J. C., Wadhwa, M., & Chattopadhyay, A. (2019). When busy is less indulging: Impact of busy mindset on self-control behaviors. Journal of Consumer Research, 45(5), 933–952.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[25] |
Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M., & Bickel, W. K. (1999). Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(1), 78–87.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[26] |
Kurtz, J. L. (2008). Looking to the future to appreciate the present: The benefits of perceived temporal scarcity. Psychological Science, 19(12), 1238–1241.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[27] |
Layous, K., Kurtz, J., Chancellor, J., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2018). Reframing the ordinary: Imagining time as scarce increases well-being. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 13(3), 301–308.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[28] |
Leary, R. B., Mesler, R. M., Simpson, B., Meng, M. D., & Montford, W. (2022). Effects of perceived scarcity on COVID-19 consumer stimulus spending: The roles of ontological insecurity and mutability in predicting prosocial outcomes. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 56(3), 1046–1061.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[29] |
Li, C., Wang, Y., Lv, X., & Li, H. (2021). To buy or not to buy? The effect of time scarcity and travel experience on tourists' impulse buying. Annals of Tourism Research, 86, 103083.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[30] |
Liu, L., Zhao, W. H., & Feng, T. Y. (2012). Cognitive mechanisms and neural basis of intertemporal selection. Psychological Science, 35(1), 56–61.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[31] |
Liu, X. S., Shi, Y., Xue, N. I., & Shen, H. (2022). The impact of time pressure on impulsive buying: The moderating role of consumption type. Tourism Management, 91, 104505.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[32] |
Malesza, M. (2019). Stress and delay discounting: The mediating role of difficulties in emotion regulation. Personality and Individual Differences, 144, 56–60.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[33] |
Mani, A., Mullainathan, S., Shafir, E., & Zhao, J. (2013). Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science, 341(6149), 976–980.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[34] |
Maule, A. J., & Hockey, G. R. J. (1993). State, stress, and time pressure. In O. Svenson & A. J. Maule (Eds.), Time Pressure and Stress in Human Judgment and Decision Making (pp. 83–101). Plenum Press.
|
[35] |
McClure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science, 306(5695), 503–507.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[36] |
Patrick, B. C., Skinner, E. A., & Connell, J. P. (1993). What motivates children's behavior and emotion? Joint effects of perceived control and autonomy in the academic domain. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 65(4), 781–791.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[37] |
Peng, J. X., Yang, Q. W., & Luo, Y. J. (2013). Selective attentional bias for different trait anxiety levels. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 45(10), 1085–1093.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[38] |
Prenda, K. M., & Lachman, M. E. (2001). Planning for the future: A life management strategy for increasing control and life satisfaction in adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 16(2), 206–216.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[39] |
Salari, N., Hosseinian-Far, A., Jalali, R., Vaisi-Raygani, A., Rasoulpoor, S., Mohammadi, M., … Khaledi-Paveh, B. (2020). Prevalence of stress, anxiety, depression among the general population during the COVID-19 pandemic: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Globalization and Health, 16(1), 1–11.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[40] |
Shah, A. K., Mullainathan, S., & Shafir, E. (2012). Some consequences of having too little. Science, 338(6107), 682–685.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[41] |
Stein, J. S., Craft, W. H., Paluch, R. A., Gatchalian, K. M., Greenawald, M. H., Quattrin, T., … Bickel, W. K. (2021). Bleak present, bright future: II. Combined effects of episodic future thinking and scarcity on delay discounting in adults at risk for type 2 diabetes. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 44, 222–230.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[42] |
Wang, C. (2020). The effect of time poverty on the cognitive decision-making of boarding elementary school students. Mental Health Education in Primary and Secondary Schools, 14, 8–14.
|
[43] |
Xia, L., Gu, R., Zhang, D., & Luo, Y. (2017). Anxious individuals are impulsive decision-makers in the delay discounting task: An ERP study. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 5.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[44] |
Xu, L., Chen, Q., Cui, N., & Lu, K.l. (2019). Enjoy the present or leave it for the future? The impact of temporal perspective on intertemporal decision-making. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(1), 96–105.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
/
〈 | 〉 |