The effect of lying on memory in daily life: Does motivation matter?

Yan Li, Zhiwei Liu

PDF
Psych Journal ›› 2024, Vol. 13 ›› Issue (2) : 216-226. DOI: 10.1002/pchj.709
ORIGINAL ARTICLE

The effect of lying on memory in daily life: Does motivation matter?

Author information +
History +

Abstract

Recently, there has been renewed interest in the effect of lying on memory. A growing body of studies has documented that lying can impair memories and cause memory disruptions, such as forgetting and false memories, to a greater degree than telling the truth. This study aimed to investigate whether motivation plays a role in the effect of lying on memory. The present study utilized a daily life paradigm and manipulated three conditions: truth telling, internally motivated lying, and externally motivated lying. We asked participants to engage in a shopping task and to tell lies (externally motivated lying group) or to choose between telling the truth (truth-telling group) and telling lies (internally motivated lying group) in the interview. Forty-eight hours later, the participants were instructed to truthfully carry out multiple memory assessments. The principal findings of this research are that lying can result in memory impairments, and internally motivated lying can lead to greater impairment in source memory than externally motivated lying. Moreover, no significant differences between the two lying groups were found in the memory tests. The empirical findings of this study provide new insights into the effect of lying on memory.

Keywords

daily life / lying / memory / motivation

Cite this article

Download citation ▾
Yan Li, Zhiwei Liu. The effect of lying on memory in daily life: Does motivation matter?. Psych Journal, 2024, 13(2): 216‒226 https://doi.org/10.1002/pchj.709

References

[1]
Abe, N. (2009). The neurobiology of deception: Evidence from neuroimaging and loss-of-function studies. Current Opinion in Neurology, 22(6), 594–600.
CrossRef Google scholar
[2]
Ackil, J. K., & Zaragoza, M. S. (2011). Forced fabrication versus interviewer suggestions: Differences in false memory depend on how memory is assessed. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(6), 933–942.
CrossRef Google scholar
[3]
Anderson, M. C., & Hanslmayr, S. (2014). Neural mechanisms of motivated forgetting. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(6), 279–292.
CrossRef Google scholar
[4]
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412.
CrossRef Google scholar
[5]
Battista, F., Curci, A., Mangiulli, I., & Otgaar, H. (2021). What can we remember after complex denials? The impact of different false denials on memory. Psychology Crime and Law, 27(9), 914–931.
CrossRef Google scholar
[6]
Battista, F., Mangiulli, I., Herter, J., Curci, A., & Otgaar, H. (2020). The effects of repeated denials and fabrication on memory. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 32(4), 369–381.
CrossRef Google scholar
[7]
Battista, F., Mangiulli, I., Riesthuis, P., Curci, A., & Otgaar, H. (2021). Do liars really remember what they lied upon? The impact of fabrication on memory. Memory, 29(7), 1–15.
CrossRef Google scholar
[8]
Battista, F., & Otgaar, H. (2022). Research on the effects of lying on memory: A Scientometric analysis and a call for new studies. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 837265.
CrossRef Google scholar
[9]
Battista, F., Otgaar, H., Mangiulli, I., & Curci, A. (2021). The role of executive functions in the effects of lying on memory. Acta Psychologica, 215, 103295.
CrossRef Google scholar
[10]
Bisby, J. A., Horner, A. J., Hørlyck, L. D., & Burgess, N. (2016). Opposing effects of negative emotion on amygdalar and hippocampal memory for items and associations. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(6), 981–990.
CrossRef Google scholar
[11]
Bücken, C. A., Mangiulli, I., Uzun, S., & Otgaar, H. (2022). False denials increase false memories for trauma-related discussions. Memory, 30(7), 1–14.
CrossRef Google scholar
[12]
Christ, S. E., Van Essen, D. C., Watson, J. M., Brubaker, L. E., & McDermott, K. B. (2009). The contributions of prefrontal cortex and executive control to deception: Evidence from activation likelihood estimate meta-analyses. Cerebral Cortex, 19(7), 1557–1566.
CrossRef Google scholar
[13]
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155–159.
CrossRef Google scholar
[14]
Elaad, E., & Gonen-Gal, Y. (2022). Face-to-face lying: Gender and motivation to deceive. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 820923.
CrossRef Google scholar
[15]
Ellis, P. D. (2010). The essential guide to effect sizes: Statistical power, meta-analysis, and the interpretation of research results. Cambridge University Press.
[16]
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.
CrossRef Google scholar
[17]
Fawcett, J. M., Lawrence, M. A., & Taylor, T. L. (2016). The representational consequences of intentional forgetting: Impairments to both the probability and fidelity of long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(1), 56–81.
CrossRef Google scholar
[18]
Frost, P., Lacroix, D., & Sanborn, N. (2003). Increasing false recognition rates with confirmatory feedback: A phenomenological analysis. American Journal of Psychology, 116(4), 515–525.
CrossRef Google scholar
[19]
Gerrie, M. P., & Garry, M. (2007). Individual differences in working memory capacity affect false memories for missing aspects of events. Memory, 15(5), 561–571.
CrossRef Google scholar
[20]
Geven, L. M., Ben-Shakhar, G., Kindt, M., & Verschuere, B. (2020). Memory-based deception detection: Extending the cognitive signature of lying from instructed to self-initiated cheating. Topics in Cognitive Science, 12(2), 608–631.
CrossRef Google scholar
[21]
Hanba, J. M., & Zaragoza, M. S. (2007). Interviewer feedback in repeated interviews involving forced confabulation. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21(4), 433–455.
CrossRef Google scholar
[22]
Krawczyk, D. C., & D'Esposito, M. (2013). Modulation of working memory function by motivation through loss-aversion. Human Brain Mapping, 34(4), 762–774.
CrossRef Google scholar
[23]
Krawczyk, D. C., Gazzaley, A., & D'Esposito, M. (2007). Reward modulation of prefrontal and visual association cortex during an incentive working memory task. Brain Research, 1141(1), 168–177.
CrossRef Google scholar
[24]
Li, Y., & Liu, Z. (2021). Involvement modulates the effects of deception on memory in daily life. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 756297.
CrossRef Google scholar
[25]
Li, Y., Liu, Z., & Liu, X. (2022a). More lies Lead to more memory impairments in daily life. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 822788.
CrossRef Google scholar
[26]
Li, Y., Liu, Z., & Liu, X. (2022b). Who did I lie to that day? Deception impairs memory in daily life. Psychological Research, 86, 1763–1773.
CrossRef Google scholar
[27]
Mangiulli, I., Lanciano, T., Jelicic, M., van Oorsouw, K., Battista, F., & Curci, A. (2018). Can implicit measures detect source information in crime-related amnesia? Memory, 26(8), 1019–1029.
CrossRef Google scholar
[28]
Mangiulli, I., Otgaar, H., Curci, A., & Jelicic, M. (2020). An experimental investigation of the misinformation effect in crime-related amnesia claims. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 34(5), 1092–1100.
CrossRef Google scholar
[29]
Mangiulli, I., Riesthuis, P., & Otgaar, H. (2022). The memory-undermining effect of simulated crime-related amnesia and its legal implications: A review. Psychological Injury and Law, 15(2), 213–226.
CrossRef Google scholar
[30]
Mangiulli, I., van Oorsouw, K., Curci, A., & Jelicic, M. (2019). Retrieval-induced forgetting in the feigning amnesia for a crime paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 10(APR), 1–10.
CrossRef Google scholar
[31]
Marsh, R. L., & Hicks, J. L. (2002). Comparisons of target output monitoring and source input monitoring. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16(7), 845–862.
CrossRef Google scholar
[32]
McArthur, J., Jarvis, R., Bourgeois, C., & Ternes, M. (2022). Lying motivations: Exploring personality correlates of lying and motivations to lie. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 54(4), 335–340.
CrossRef Google scholar
[33]
Otgaar, H., & Baker, A. (2018). When lying changes memory for the truth. Memory, 26(1), 2–14.
CrossRef Google scholar
[34]
Otgaar, H., Howe, M. L., Mangiulli, I., & Bücken, C. (2020). The impact of false denials on forgetting and false memory. Cognition, 202, 104322.
CrossRef Google scholar
[35]
Otgaar, H., Howe, M. L., Smeets, T., & Wang, J. (2016). Denial-induced forgetting: False denials undermine memory, but external denials undermine belief. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5(2), 168–175.
CrossRef Google scholar
[36]
Otgaar, H., Romeo, T., Ramakers, N., & Howe, M. L. (2018). Forgetting having denied: The “amnesic” consequences of denial. Memory and Cognition, 46(4), 520–529.
CrossRef Google scholar
[37]
Palena, N., Caso, L., Cavagnis, L., Greco, A., & Vrij, A. (2022). Exploring the relationship between personality, morality and lying: A study based on the person-centred approach. Current Psychology, 1–14, 20502–20514.
CrossRef Google scholar
[38]
Peters, M. J. V., Jelicic, M., Verbeek, H., & Merckelbach, H. (2007). Poor working memory predicts false memories. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19(2), 213–232.
CrossRef Google scholar
[39]
Pezdek, K., Lam, S. T., & Sperry, K. (2009). Forced confabulation more strongly influences event memory if suggestions are other-generated than self-generated. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 14(2), 241–252.
CrossRef Google scholar
[40]
Phelps, E. A. (2004). Human emotion and memory: Interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(2), 198–202.
CrossRef Google scholar
[41]
Polage, D. C. (2019). Liar, liar: Consistent lying decreases belief in the truth. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33(4), 527–536.
CrossRef Google scholar
[42]
Riesthuis, P., Mangiulli, I., Bogaard, G., & Otgaar, H. (2022). The impact of fabrication on recognition memory: An experimental study. New Ideas in Psychology, 67(100966), 100966.
CrossRef Google scholar
[43]
Riesthuis, P., Otgaar, H., Battista, F., & Mangiulli, I. (2021). Public beliefs on the relationship between lying and memory. Psychology Crime and Law, 27, 545–568.
CrossRef Google scholar
[44]
Riesthuis, P., Otgaar, H., Hope, L., & Mangiulli, I. (2022). The effects of incentivized lies on memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 1, 1–12.
CrossRef Google scholar
[45]
Riesthuis, P., Otgaar, H., Hope, L., & Mangiulli, I. (2022). The effects of repetition on memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 36(4), 1–13.
[46]
Riesthuis, P., Otgaar, H., Mangiulli, I., & de Tauzia, R. (2020). Adopting a fictitious autobiography: Fabrication inflation or deflation? Memory, 28(6), 741–752.
CrossRef Google scholar
[47]
Romeo, T., Otgaar, H., Smeets, T., Landstrom, S., & Boerboom, D. (2019). The impact of lying about a traumatic virtual reality experience on memory. Memory and Cognition, 47(3), 485–495.
CrossRef Google scholar
[48]
Romeo, T., Otgaar, H., Smeets, T., Landström, S., & Jelicic, M. (2019). The memory-impairing effects of simulated amnesia for a mock crime. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33(6), 983–990.
CrossRef Google scholar
[49]
Shen, Q., Teo, M., Winter, E., Hart, E., Chew, S. H., & Ebstein, R. P. (2016). To cheat or not to cheat: Tryptophan hydroxylase 2 SNP variants contribute to dishonest behavior. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 10, 82.
CrossRef Google scholar
[50]
Sneddon, A. (2021). Alternative motivation and lies. Analysis (United Kingdom), 81(1), 46–52.
CrossRef Google scholar
[51]
Suchotzki, K., Verschuere, B., Van Bockstaele, B., Ben-Shakhar, G., & Crombez, G. (2017). Lying takes time: A meta-analysis on reaction time measures of deception. Psychological Bulletin, 143(4), 428–453.
CrossRef Google scholar
[52]
Taylor, S. F., Welsh, R. C., D.Wager, T., Phan, K. L., Fitzgerald, K. D., & Gehring, W. J. (2004). A tional neuroimaging study of motivation and executive function. NeuroImage, 21, 1045–1054.
CrossRef Google scholar
[53]
van Oorsouw, K., & Giesbrecht, T. (2008). Minimizing culpability increases commission errors in a mock crime paradigm. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 13(2), 335–344.
CrossRef Google scholar
[54]
van Oorsouw, K., & Merckelbach, H. (2004). Feigning amnesia undermines memory for a mock crime. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(5), 505–518.
CrossRef Google scholar
[55]
Vieira, K. M., & Lane, S. M. (2013). How you lie affects what you remember. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 2(3), 173–178.
CrossRef Google scholar
[56]
Walczyk, J., Harris, L. L., Duck, T. K., & Mulay, D. (2014). A social-cognitive framework for understanding serious lies: Activation-decision-construction-action theory. New Ideas in Psychology, 34(1), 22–36.
CrossRef Google scholar
[57]
Wechsler, D. (1997). WAIS-III administration and scoring manual. The Psychological Corporation.
[58]
Zang, C., Fu, Y., Bai, X., Yan, G., & Liversedge, S. P. (2021). Foveal and parafoveal processing of Chinese three-character idioms in reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 119(1), 104243.
CrossRef Google scholar
[59]
Zaragoza, M. S., Payment, K. E., Ackil, J. K., Drivdahl, S. B., & Beck, M. (2001). Interviewing witnesses: Forced confabulation and confirmatory feedback increase false memories. Psychological Science, 12(6), 473–477.
CrossRef Google scholar

RIGHTS & PERMISSIONS

2023 2023 The Authors. PsyCh Journal published by Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences and John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd.
PDF

Accesses

Citations

Detail

Sections
Recommended

/