Self-truncated sampling produces more moderate covariation judgment and related decision than descriptive frequency information: The role of regressive frequency estimation
Xuhui Zhang, Junyi Dai
Self-truncated sampling produces more moderate covariation judgment and related decision than descriptive frequency information: The role of regressive frequency estimation
Covariation judgment underlies a diversity of psychological theories and influences various everyday decisions. Information about covariation can be learned from either a summary description of frequencies (i.e., contingency tables) or trial-by-trial experience (i.e., sampling individual instances). Two studies were conducted to investigate the impact of information learning mode (i.e., description vs. self-truncated sampling) on covariation judgment and related decision. In each trial under the description condition, participants were presented with a contingency table with explicit cell frequencies, whereas in each trial under the self-truncated sampling condition, participants were allowed to determine when to stop sampling instances and thus the actual sample size. To eliminate sampling error, an other-yoked (i.e., between-subject) design was used in this research so that cell frequencies shown in a trial under the description condition were matched with those experienced in a trial under the self-truncated sampling condition. Experiment 1 showed that the self-truncated sampling condition led to more moderate covariation judgments than the description condition (i.e., a description–experience gap). Experiment 2 demonstrated further that the same gap extended to covariation-related decisions in terms of relative contingent preference (RCP). Regressive frequency estimation under self-truncated sampling appeared to underlie the consistent gaps found in the two studies, whereas the impact of regressive diagnosticity (i.e., the same sample of instances was viewed as less diagnostic under description than under self-truncated sampling) or simultaneous overestimation and underweighting of rare instances under experience was not supported by the observed data. Future research might examine alternative accounts of the observed gaps, such as the impacts of belief updating and stopping rule under self-truncated sampling.
contingency table / covariation judgment / decision / regressive frequency estimation / self-truncated sampling
[1] |
Alloy, L. B., & Tabachnik, N. (1984). Assessment of covariation by humans and animals: The joint influence of prior expectations and current situational information. Psychological Review, 91, 112–149.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[2] |
Arkes, H. R., & Harkness, A. R. (1983). Estimates of contingency between two dichotomous variables. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112(1), 117–135.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[3] |
Barron, G., & Yechiam, E. (2009). The coexistence of overestimation and underweighting of rare events and the contingent recency effect. Judgment and Decision Making, 4(6), 447–460.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[4] |
Beyth-Marom, R. (1982). Perception of correlation reexamined. Memory & Cognition, 10(6), 511–519.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[5] |
Bott, F. M., & Meiser, T. (2020). Pseudocontingency inference and choice: The role of information sampling. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(9), 1624–1644.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[6] |
Bürkner, P. (2017). Brms: An R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan. Journal of Statistical Software, 80(1), 1–28.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[7] |
Camilleri, A. R., & Newell, B. R. (2011). When and why rare events are underweighted: A direct comparison of the sampling, partial feedback, full feedback and description choice paradigms. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(2), 377–384.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[8] |
Chapman, L. J., & Chapman, J. P. (1969). Illusory correlation as an obstacle to the use of valid psychodiagnostic signs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 74(3), 271–280.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[9] |
Crocker, J. (1981). Judgment of covariation by social perceivers. Psychological Bulletin, 90(2), 272–292.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[10] |
Fantino, E., & Navarro, A. (2012). Description–experience gaps: Assessments in other choice paradigms. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25(3), 303–314.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[11] |
Fiedler, K. (2010). Pseudocontingencies can override genuine contingencies between multiple cues. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(4), 504–509.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[12] |
Fiedler, K., & Freytag, P. (2004). Pseudocontingencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 453–467.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[13] |
Fiedler, K., Freytag, P., & Meiser, T. (2009). Pseudocontingencies: An integrative account of an intriguing cognitive illusion. Psychological Review, 116(1), 187–206.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[14] |
Fiedler, K., & Kareev, Y. (2006). Does decision quality (always) increase with the size of information samples? Some vicissitudes in applying the law of large numbers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(4), 883–903.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[15] |
Fiedler, K., Renn, S.-Y., & Kareev, Y. (2010). Mood and judgments based on sequential sampling. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 23(5), 483–495.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[16] |
Fiedler, K., & Unkelbach, C. (2014). Regressive judgment: Implications of a universal property of the empirical world. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(5), 361–367.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[17] |
Fleig, H., Meiser, T., Ettlin, F., & Rummel, J. (2017). Statistical numeracy as a moderator of (pseudo)contingency effects on decision behavior. Acta Psychologica, 174, 68–79.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[18] |
Glöckner, A., Hilbig, B. E., Henninger, F., & Fiedler, S. (2016). The reversed description-experience gap: Disentangling sources of presentation format effects in risky choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(4), 486–508.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[19] |
Hamilton, D. L., Dugan, P. M., & Trolier, T. K. (1985). The formation of stereotypic beliefs: Further evidence for distinctiveness-based illusory correlations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(1), 5–17.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[20] |
Hamilton, D. L., & Gifford, R. (1976). Illusory correlations in interpersonal perception: A cognitive basis of stereotypic judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12, 392–407.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[21] |
Hamilton, D. L., & Rose, T. L. (1980). Illusory correlation and the maintenance of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(5), 832–845.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[22] |
Hertwig, R., Barron, G., Weber, E. U., & Erev, I. (2004). Decisions from experience and the effect of rare events in risky choice. Psychological Science, 15(8), 534–539.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[23] |
Hogarth, R. M., & Einhorn, H. J. (1992). Order effects in belief updating: The belief-adjustment model. Cognitive Psychology, 24(1), 1–55.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[24] |
JASP Team. (2023). JASP (Version 0.17.1)[Computer software].
|
[25] |
Jenkins, H. M., & Ward, W. C. (1965). Judgment of contingency between responses and outcomes. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 79(1), 1–17.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[26] |
Kao, S.-F., & Wasserman, E. A. (1993). Assessment of an information integration account of contingency judgment with examination of subjective cell importance and method of information presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(6), 1363–1386.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[27] |
Kruschke, J. K., & Liddell, T. M. (2018). Bayesian data analysis for newcomers. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(1), 155–177.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[28] |
Lindley, D. V. (1965). Introduction to probability and statistics from a Bayesian point of view, part 2: Inference. Cambridge University Press.
|
[29] |
Mata, A. (2016). Judgment of covariation: A review. Psicologia, 30(1), 61–74.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[30] |
Mercier, P., & Parr, W. (1996). Inter-trial interval, stimulus duration and number of trials in contingency judgments. British Journal of Psychology, 87(4), 549–566.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[31] |
Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Prentice-Hall.
|
[32] |
Prager, J., & Fiedler, K. (2021). Forming impressions from self-truncated sampling of traits – Interplay of Thurstonian and Brunswikian sampling effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 121, 474–497.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[33] |
Prager, J., Krueger, J. I., & Fiedler, K. (2018). Towards a deeper understanding of impression formation-new insights gained from a cognitive-ecological perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 115(3), 379–397.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[34] |
R Core Team. (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing URL
|
[35] |
Rouder, J. N., Speckman, P. L., Sun, D., Morey, R. D., & Iverson, G. (2009). Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(2), 225–237.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[36] |
Seggie, I. (1987). The judgment of covariation between binary variables: Some conditions that influence the process. Memory & Cognition, 15(4), 341–348.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[37] |
Seggie, J. L., & Endersby, H. (1972). The empirical implications of piaget's concept of correlation. Australian Journal of Psychology, 24(1), 3–8.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[38] |
Shaklee, H. (1983). Human covariation judgment: Accuracy and strategy. Learning and Motivation, 14, 433–448.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[39] |
Shaklee, H., & Mims, M. (1982). Sources of error in judging event covariations: Effects of memory demands. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8(3), 208–224.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[40] |
Shaklee, H., & Tucker, D. (1980). A rule analysis of judgments of covariation between events. Memory & Cognition, 8(5), 459–467.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[41] |
Smedslund, J. (1963). The concept of correlation in adults. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 4(1), 165–173.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[42] |
Szollosi, A., Liang, G., Konstantinidis, E., Donkin, C., & Newell, B. R. (2019). Simultaneous underweighting and overestimation of rare events: Unpacking a paradox. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(12), 2207–2217.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[43] |
Tingley, D., Yamamoto, T., Hirose, K., Keele, L., & Imai, K. (2014). Mediation: R package for causal mediation analysis. Journal of Statistical Software, 59, 1–38.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[44] |
Vogel, T., Freytag, P., Kutzner, F., & Fiedler, K. (2013). Pseudocontingencies derived from categorically organized memory representations. Memory & Cognition, 41, 1185–1199.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[45] |
Ward, W. C., & Jenkins, H. M. (1965). The display of information and the judgment of contingency. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 19(3), 231–241.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[46] |
Wasserman, E. A., Dorner, W. W., & Kao, S.-F. (1990). Contributions of specific cell information to judgments of interevent contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 509–521.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[47] |
Wulff, D. U., Mergenthaler-Canseco, M., & Hertwig, R. (2018). A meta-analytic review of two modes of learning and the description-experience gap. Psychological Bulletin, 144(2), 140–176.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[48] |
Wulff, D. U., & Pachur, T. (2016). Modeling valuations from experience: A comment on Ashby and Rakow (2014). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(1), 158–166.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[49] |
Yates, J. F., & Curley, S. P. (1986). Contingency judgment: Primacy effects and attention decrement. Acta Psychologica, 62(3), 293–302.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[50] |
Yechiam, E., & Busemeyer, J. R. (2006). The effect of foregone payoffs on underweighting small probability events. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 1–16.
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
/
〈 | 〉 |