Addiction as a dynamical rationality disorder
Hava T. SIEGELMANN
Addiction as a dynamical rationality disorder
Addiction is frequently modeled as a behavioral disorder resulting from the internal battle between two subsystems: one model describes slow planning versus fast habitual action; another, hot versus cold modes. In another model, one subsystem pushes the individual toward substance abuse, while the other tries to pull him away. These models all describe one side winning over the other at each point of confrontation, represented as a simple binary switch: on or off, win or lose. We propose however, an alternative model, in which opposing systems work in parallel, tipping toward one subsystem or the other, in greater or lesser degree, based on a continuous rationality factor. Our approach results in a dynamical system that qualitatively emulates seeking behavior, cessation, and relapse—enabling the accurate description of a process that can lead to recovery. As an adjunct to the model, we are in the process of creating an associated, interactive website that will enable addicts to journal their thoughts, emotions and actions on a daily basis. The site is not only a potentially rich source of data for our model, but will in its primary function aid addicts to individually identify parameters affecting their decisions and behavior.
addiction / emotional-cognitive rationality / dynamical systems / dynamical disease / dynamical rehabilitation / relapse / higher power
[1] |
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Results from the 2007 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: National Findings. NSDUH Series H-34, DHHS Publication No. SMA 08–4343, 2008
|
[2] |
Kreek M J, Nielsen D A, Butelman E R, LaForge K S. Genetic influences on impulsivity, risk taking, stress responsivity and vulnerability to drug abuse and addiction. Nature Neuroscience, 2005, 8(11): 1450-1457
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[3] |
Volkow N D, Fowler J S, Wang G J. The addicted human brain viewed in the light of imaging studies: brain circuits and treatment strategies. Neuropharmacology, 2004, 47(Suppl 1): 3-13
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[4] |
Jones A M. Health, addiction, social interaction and the decision to quit smoking. Journal of Health Economics, 1994, 13(1): 93-110
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[5] |
Kleber H D, Weissman M M, Rounsaville B J, Wilber C H, Prusoff B A, Riordan C E. Imipramine as treatment for depression in addicts. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1983, 40(6): 649-653
|
[6] |
Redish A D. Addiction as a computational process gone awry. Science, 2004, 306(5703): 1944-1947
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[7] |
Heyman G M. Addiction: a Disorder of Choice. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 2009
|
[8] |
Soria G, Barbano M F, Maldonado R, Valverde O. A reliable method to study cue-, priming-, and stress-induced reinstatement of cocaine self-administration in mice. Psychopharmacology, 2008, 199(4): 593-603
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[9] |
Johnson P M, Kenny P J. Dopamine D2 receptors in addiction-like reward dysfunction and compulsive eating in obese rats. Nature Neuroscience, 2010, 13(5): 635-641
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[10] |
Alexander B K. “The Myth of Drug-Induced Addiction”, a paper delivered to the Canadian Senate, 2001
|
[11] |
Dockner E J, Feichtinger G. Cyclical consumption patterns and rational addiction. American Economic Review, 1993, 83(1): 256-263
|
[12] |
Epstein D H, Willner-Reid J, Vahabzadeh M, Mezghanni M, Lin J L, Preston K L. Real-time electronic diary reports of cue exposure and mood in the hours before cocaine and heroin craving and use. Archives of General Psychiatry, 2009, 66(1): 88-94
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[13] |
Goldstein R Z, Volkow N D. Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. American Journal of Psychiatry, 2002, 159(10): 1642-1652
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[14] |
McClure S M, Daw N D, Montague P R. A computational substrate for incentive salience. Trends in Neurosciences, 2003, 26(8): 423-428
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[15] |
Ahmed S H, Koob G F. Transition to drug addiction: a negative reinforcement model based on an allostatic decrease in reward function. Psychopharmacology, 2005, 180(3): 473-490
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[16] |
Berridge K C, Robinson T E. Parsing reward. Trends in Neurosciences, 2003, 26(9): 507-513
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[17] |
Redish A D, Jensen S, Johnson A, Kurth-Nelson Z. Reconciling reinforcement learning models with behavioral extinction and renewal: implications for addiction, relapse, and problem gambling. Psychological Review, 2007, 114(3): 784-805
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[18] |
Dezfouli A, Piray P, Keramati M M, Ekhtiari H, Lucas C, Mokri A. A neurocomputational model for cocaine addiction. Neural Computation, 2009, 21(10): 2869-2893
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[19] |
Redish A D, Jensen S, Johnson A. A unified framework for addiction: vulnerabilities in the decision process. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2008, 31(4): 415-487
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[20] |
Daw N D, Niv Y, Dayan P. Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control. Nature Neuroscience, 2005, 8(12): 1704-1711
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[21] |
Redish A D, Johnson A. A computational model of craving and obsession. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 2007, 1104(1): 324-339
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[22] |
Bickel W K, Yi R. Addiction science as a hedgehog and as a fox. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2008, 31(4): 441-442
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[23] |
Ahmed S H, Graupner M, Gutkin B. Computational approaches to the neurobiology of drug addiction. Pharmacopsychiatry, 2009, 42(Suppl 1): S144-S152
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[24] |
Gutkin B S, Dehaene S, Changeux J P. A neurocompu tational hypothesis for nicotine addiction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 2006, 103(4): 1106-1111
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[25] |
Becker G S, Murphy K M. A theory of rational addiction. Journal of Political Economy, 1988, 96(4): 675-699
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[26] |
Bernheim B D, Rangel A. Addiction and cue-triggered decision processes. American Economic Review, 2004, 94(5): 1558-1590
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[27] |
Winick C. Maturing out of narcotic addiction. Bulletin on Narcotics, 1962, 14(1): 1-7
|
[28] |
Maddux J F, Desmond D P. New light on the maturing out hypothesis in opioid dependence. Bulletin on Narcotics, 1980, 32(1): 15-25
|
[29] |
Waldorf D. Natural recovery from opiate addiction: some socio-psychological processes of untreated recovery. Journal of Drug Issues, 1983, 13(2): 237-280
|
[30] |
Tucker J A. Different pathways to knowledge about different pathways to recovery: a comment on the people awakening study. Addiction, 2008, 103(2): 216-217
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[31] |
Alligood K T, Sauer T, Yorke J A. Chaos, An Introduction to Dynamical Systems. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1997
|
[32] |
Leise T, Siegelmann H. Dynamics of a multistage circadian system. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 2006, 21(4): 314-323
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[33] |
Rapatski B L, Suppe F, Yorke J A. HIV epidemics driven by late disease stage transmission. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes, 2005, 38(3): 241-253
|
[34] |
Olsen M M, Siegelmann-Danieli N, Siegelmann H T. Dynamic computational model suggests that cellular citizenship is fundamental for selective tumor apoptosis. PloS ONE, 2010, 5(5): e10637
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[35] |
Glass L, Mackey MC. From Clocks to Chaos: The Rhythms of Life. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1988
|
[36] |
Belair J, Glass L, An Der Heiden U, Milton J. Dynamical disease: identification, temporal aspects and treatment strategies of human illness. Chaos, 1995, 5(1): 1-7
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[37] |
Siegelmann H T. Complex systems science and brain dynamics. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 2010, 4(7): 1-2
|
[38] |
Anthony J C, Warner L A, Kessler R C. Comparative epidemiology of dependence on tobacco, alcohol, controlled substances, and inhalants: basic findings from the national comorbidity survey. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 1994, 2(3): 244-268
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[39] |
Koenigs M, Young L, Adolphs R, Tranel D, Cushman F, Hauser M, Damasio A. Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements. Nature, 2007, 446(7138): 908-911
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[40] |
Monterosso J, Ainslie G. The behavioral economics of will in recovery from addiction. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 2007, 90(Suppl 1): S100-S111
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
[41] |
Niv Y, Joel D, Dayan P. A normative perspective on motivation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2006, 10(8): 375-381
CrossRef
Google scholar
|
/
〈 | 〉 |